Rol EI in Education and Programs
Assessment and Education of Emotional Intelligence
Topic 4 - The role of EI in
education
Part
I. Take-away topic questions
Why should EI be expected to be a strong predictor of academic performance?
And why not?
Empirical evidence supports the notion that EI impacts academic success
either directly or by supporting key personal resources: individuals who score
higher in EI have better academic adjustment, probably because they’re better
at coping with stress and keeping themselves motivated, and have better social
support.
Cognitive ability remains by and large the main predictor of academic
achievement, however, and socioeconomic status also plays a very important
role. It could be argued therefore that interventions targeting EI are missing
the point, since the contribution of EI to academic performance will always be
marginal.
According to Petrides et al.’s (2004) study, what was the relationship
between trait EI and academic performance for students with lower IQs?
Because they experience more stress during their studies, adolescents with
low IQ benefit academically if they have appropriate self-perceived emotional
skills, since EI informs the behavioural response to the stressor, thus
moderating the effect of cognitive ability.
How would you summarize Qualter et al.’s conclusions about future research
on EI and education?
There needs to be rigorous scientific evaluation of the interventions
aiming at developing EI in students. To do so, a multi-method approach to
measuring EI must be adopted, and evaluations must seek to explain why
intervention is effective under certain circumstances, or for certain types of
children, but not others.
Assessment and
Education of Emotional Intelligence
Topic 5 - Emotional education programmes
Part I. Take-away topic
questions
According to Schutte et al.’s (2013) article, Nelis et al.’s (2009)
article, and Mikolajczak & Peña Sarrionandia’s (2015) article, what does
the evidence say about improving EI through training/education programmes?
·
both ability and trait EI assessed at the
end of the programmes show significant improvements
·
positive consequences associated to higher
EI (better perceived physical and mental health, higher relationship and work
satisfaction, for instance) are also observed
·
biological markers of health also tend to
improve
·
some studies have carried out follow-up
measurements and have found that the changes tend to persist
·
hypothesised mechanisms underlying the
improvement in EI:
o
training influences ability and trait
emotional intelligence directly, through expanding an individual’s
emotion-related knowledge and skills
o
training mainly impacts emotional
self-efficacy, which leads over time to practice-related changes in emotional
intelligence ability and trait emotional intelligence
·
more research is required to verify these
initial findings and to uncover how training increases emotional intelligence,
what specific training works best, and what important outcomes can be produced
Part II. Critical
reading questions
Is there a compromise amongst researchers in the field of EI regarding
the different constructs of ability and trait EI and their usefulness when
designing and evaluating educational interventions?
Some of the essential readings for this topic seem to suggest a
preference for a multi-method approach when it comes to applied settings: both
Schutte et al. (2013), on the one hand, and Mikolajczak, Petrides et al. (2009,
cited in Mikolajczak & Peña-Sarrionandia, 2015), on the other hand, take
into account different aspects of emotional competency, as I have tried to
summarise in the table below.
Mikolajczak,
Petrides et al. (2009, cited in Mikolajczak & Peña-Sarrionandia, 2015) |
Schutte
et al. (2013) |
|
declarative knowledge what people know about emotions and emotionally intelligent behaviors
(e.g. Do I know which emotional expressions are constructive in a given
social situation?) |
assessed with maximum-performance tests such as the MSCEIT (it’s the only one which covers the domain comprehensively, yet its
validity has been questioned) |
|
ability (AEI) the ability to apply this knowledge in a real-world situation (e.g. Am I able to express my emotions constructively in a
given social situation?) |
||
trait (TEI) emotion-related behaviour and dispositions. The focus here is not on what people know or on what they are
able to do, but on what they typically do over extensive periods of time |
measured via self-report questionnaires (TEIQ, Bar-On/EQi) |
|
self-efficacy (ESE) beliefs about one’s own emotional competency |
measured via self-report questionnaires tapping respondents’ beliefs
about their emotional competency, such as the Emotional Self-Efficacy Scale
(Kirk, Schutte & Hine, 2008) |
These layers would be loosely connected, that is, they wouldn’t
necessarily correlate -we’ve learnt already that correlations between ability
EI measures and trait EI measures are weak. According to Qualter et al. (2017),
they’re different ways of operationalising emotional competency, neither
interchangeable nor mutually exclusive, which capture a unique slice of a
person’s overall emotional profile and should be measured independently, using
different instruments. Moreover, they are all influenced when targeted by
educational intervention and they contribute independently to mental health and
academic outcomes.
Notwithstanding the addition of ESE as distinct from TEI, a fundamental
split between actual ability and perception of ability still stands.
What are the benefits of adopting a multi-method approach?
1. The strengths of one method offset the limitations of another:
o
self-report measures are susceptible to
self-presentation bias and to poor self-insight, which means they’re not an
appropriate tool for measuring actual ability. However, they’re the only way to
access highly subjective phenomena such as ESE
o
maximum-performance tests can’t be scored
objectively; when the correct answer is determined by consensus or expert
judgement high scores may reflect declarative knowledge or conformity to social
norms, instead of emotional ability
2. It can reveal within-person discrepancies:
o
some people may be quite skilled
emotionally (high AEI), but lack the self-confidence to translate those skills
into everyday behaviour (low TEI/ESE), others may believe they are quite
competent (high TEI/ESE), yet their actual abilities might be lacking (low
AEI). Both these discrepancy profiles have been linked to poorer
behavioural and well-being outcomes.
o
there’s also evidence that high TEI/ESE
may offset the effects of low AEI for some individuals
o
there's evidence of decreased TEI after an
EI training programme, which can be interpreted as an individual with an
initially discrepant profile who gains a more accurate self-perception
as a result of the training in emotional competence, which should be seen as an
improvement in my opinion
All in all, it’s essential that an intervention is designed with a
specific construct of emotional competency in mind, which will determine the
measurements that should be used to monitor and assess progress, and the
appropriateness of the programme for a given context.
What are the mechanisms underlying EI improvement due to training?
Given that EI is a multifaceted construct, it seems necessary to ask
what aspects of it we’re targeting in EE programmes, what mechanisms we expect
to bring about change, and whether there's interaction between the different
aspects.
According to Schutte et al. (2013), training most likely directly
influences both AEI and TEI through expanding an individual’s emotion-related
knowledge and skills. Alternatively, it might be that training mainly impacts
ESE, and that an increase in confidence leads over time to practice-related
changes in AEI and TEI.
In my opinion, these two different mechanisms should yield different
measurement results: training-related ESE improvement should have a greater
effect on AEI and TEI measures after some time has passed since the end of the
intervention.
Comentarios
Publicar un comentario